Influence of alcohol on the vigilance levels for a driving test

  • Etobo Innocent AHOUNOU Laboratoire de Neurosciences, UFR Biosciences, Université Félix Houphouët-Boigny, 22 BP 582 Abidjan 22, République de Côte d’Ivoire
  • Koffi Mathias YAO Laboratoire de Neurosciences, UFR Biosciences, Université Félix Houphouët-Boigny, 22 BP 582 Abidjan 22, République de Côte d’Ivoire
  • Kobenan Fiéni Jean-Baptiste ADOU Laboratoire de Neurosciences, UFR Biosciences, Université Félix Houphouët-Boigny, 22 BP 582 Abidjan 22, République de Côte d’Ivoire
  • Némé Antoine TAKO Laboratoire de Neurosciences, UFR Biosciences, Université Félix Houphouët-Boigny, 22 BP 582 Abidjan 22, République de Côte d’Ivoire
  • Bialli SERI Laboratoire de Neurosciences, UFR Biosciences, Université Félix Houphouët-Boigny, 22 BP 582 Abidjan 22, République de Côte d’Ivoire
Keywords: Alertness, alcohol, acute consumption, errors

Abstract

In order to determine the maximum alcohol to remain sufficiently vigilant during a driving, sixty (60) male volunteers divided into two groups of thirty, including a control group and an experimental group previously submitted to acute consumption of alcohol, were chosen and participated in a driving test. Each subject shall drive a vehicle on a straight track for 500 meters with a circular terminal after 80 meters, enabling him to return to the starting point. The speed was imposed by an instructor and obstacles (balls) may be set to arise on the way. Ten centimeters were added on the side edges of the vehicle to plot the width of the road ahead. Thus, any excess of the lateral edges is a lateral error and any ball reached, arising in front of the driver, is a frontal error. The test is performed each hour for five consecutive hours. The results indicated that the number of errors increases with speed, the side that errors outnumber frontal errors and the acceptable blood alcohol for driving test would be 0.3 g of alcohol per liter of blood. Thus, the state should reduce the permitted blood alcohol, currently at 0.8 g / l, at a rate less than or equal to 0.3 g / l. The state must also equip the police forces of devices adapted to make systematic control

References

Alvarez FJ, Del Rio MC. (2003). Response to Ramaekers: pitfalls in estimating drug related crash risk. Trends in Pharmacological Sciences, 24(3):115, .

Pfelbaum AM, Romon M, Dubus M. (2009). Diététique et Nutrition, 7th ed. Elsevier Masson, Paris.

Ben-Amar M. (2007). Les psychotropes criminogènes. Presses Univ. Montréal, 40(1):11-30.

Brady JE, Li G (2013). Prevalence of alcohol and other drugs in fatally injured drivers. Addiction, 108(1):104–114, .
Campton RP, Blomberg RD, Moscowitz H, Burns M, Peek RC, Fiorentino D. (2002). Crash risk of alcohol impaired driving. In: Mayhew DR, Dussault C, eds. Proceedings of the 16th International Conference on Alcohol, Drugs and Traffic Safety, Montreal, 4–9 August 2002. Montreal, Société de l’assurance automobile du Québec, 39–44.

Deery HA, Love AW. (1996). The effect of a moderate dose of alcohol on the traffic hazard perception profile of young drink-drivers. Addiction, 91(6):815-827.

Dupont RL, Voas RB, Walsh JM, Shea C, Talpins SK, Neil MM. (2012). The need for drugged driving per se laws: a commentary. Traffic Injury Prevention, 13(1):31–42.

Elvik R, Mysen AB, Vaa T. (1997). Trafi kksikkerhetshåndbok, tredje utgave [Handbook of traffi c safety, 3e edition, Oslo (Norvège), Institute of Transport Economics.

Ettorre E. (2004). Revisioning women and drug use: gender sensitivity, embodiment and reducing harm. International Journal of Drug Policy, 15(5-6):327-335.

Finch DJ, Kompfner P, Lockwood CR, Maycock G. (1994). Speed, speed limits and accidents. Crowthorne (Royaume-Uni), Transport Research Laboratory, Project Report 58.

Girre C, Hirschhorn M, Bertaux L, Palombo S, Fournier PE. (1991). Comparison of performance of healthy volunteers given prazepam alone or combined with ethanol relation to drug plasma concentrations. International Clinical Psychopharmacology, 6(4):227-238.

[INSERM] Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale. (2001). Alcool : effets sur la santé. Expertise collective ; France, Septembre, 119-129 ; 267-273.

Jones AW, Jönsson KA, Kechagias S. (1997). Effect of high-fat, high-protein, and highcarbohydrate meals on the pharmacokinetics of a small dose of alcohol. British Journal of Clinical Pharmacology, 44(6):521-526.

Kelly E, Darke S, Ross J. (2004). A review of drug use and driving: epidemiology, impairment, risk factors and risk perceptions. Drug and Alcohol Review, 23(3):319–344.

Koelega HS. (1995). Alcohol and vigilance performance: a review. Psychopharmacology, 118(3):233-249.

Lyvers MF, Maltzman I. (1991). Selective effects of alcohol on Wisconsin card sorting test performance. British Journal of Addiction, 86(4):399-407.

McLean J, Kloeden C. (2002). Alcohol, travelling speed and the risk of crash involvement. In: Mayhew DR, Dussault C, eds. Actes de la 16e Conférence internationale sur l’alcool, les drogues et la sécurité routière, Montréal, 4–9 août 2002. Montréal (Canada), Société de l’assurance automobile du Québec, 73–79.

Moskowitz HA, Fiorentino D. (2000). A review of the literature on the effects of low doses of alcohol on driving-related skills. Washington, DC: US National Highway Traffic Safety Administration, Report n°. DOT HS 809 028, April

Mura P. (1999). Alcool, médicaments, stupéfiants et conduite automobile. ed. Elsevier Paris.

O’Boyle DJ, Binns AS, Sumner JJ. (1994). On the efficacy of alcohol placebos in inducing feelings of intoxication. Psychopharmacology, 115(1-2):229-236.

[OMS] Organisation Mondiale de la Santé. (2006). Problèmes de santé publique imputables à l'abus d'alcool. Cinquante-septième session du bureau régional du pacifique occidental ; point 12; 18 au 22 septembre.

Oscar-Berman M, Marinkovic K. (2007). Alcohol: Effects on neurobehavioral functions and the brain. Neuropsychol Review, 17(3):239-257.

Perry BL. (2004). Premenstrual symptomatology and alcohol consumption. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs, 65(4):464-468.

Peterson JB, Rothfleisch J, Zelazo PD, Pihl RO. (1990). Acute alcohol intoxication and cognitive functioning. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs, 51(2):114-122.

Ruitenberg A, Den Heijer T, Bakker SL, Van Swieten JC, Koudstaal PJ, Hofman A, Breteler MM. (2005). Cerebral hypoperfusion and clinical onset of dementia: the Rotterdam Study. Annals of Neurology, 57(6):789-794.

Sano M, Wendt PE, Wirsen A, Stenberg G, Risberg J, Ingvar DH. (1993). Acute effects of alcohol on regional cerebral blood flow in man. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drug, 54(3):369-376.

Taylor MC, Baruya A, Kennedy JV. (2002). The relationship between speed and accidents on rural single-carriageway roads. Crowthorne (Royaume-Uni), Transport Research Laboratory, Rapport 511 du TR.

West R, Wilding J, French D, Kemp R, Irving A. (1993). Effect of low and moderate doses of alcohol on driving hazard perception latency and driving speed. Addiction, 88(4):527-532.

Widmark EMP. (1932). Die theorischen Grundladen und die praktische Verwendarbeit der gerichtlichmedizinischen Alkohol Bestimmung. Fortsch Naturw Forshung, 11:140.

Yao KM, Camara PA, Adou KFJB. (2012). Types de boissons alcooliques consommées en Côte d’Ivoire. Préférence et consommation effective. Alcoologie et Addictologie, 34(3):185-193.
Published
2015-12-15
How to Cite
AHOUNOU, E. I., YAO, K. M., ADOU, K. F. J.-B., TAKO, N. A., & SERI, B. (2015). Influence of alcohol on the vigilance levels for a driving test. Journal of Research in Biology, 5(7), 1874-1884. Retrieved from https://ojs.jresearchbiology.com/ojs1/index.php/jrb/article/view/402